Skip to main content

Spectrum: Autism Research News

Jokes crack open brain connectivity in autism

by  /  28 September 2010
THIS ARTICLE IS MORE THAN FIVE YEARS OLD

This article is more than five years old. Autism research — and science in general — is constantly evolving, so older articles may contain information or theories that have been reevaluated since their original publication date.

Funny business: Charting how the brain responds to humor may help understand some of the neurological deficits in people with autism.

Why did the rooster cross the road? To prove he wasn’t a chicken.

Humor, even in the form of silly jokes like the one above, is known to be critical for social and cognitive development. Telling jokes allows children to connect with others, refine their language skills and develop keen imaginations.

Because these are precisely the skills lacking in people with autism, studying humor in children with the disorder may give insights into their abnormal brain circuitry and even lead to therapies, according to a review published in June in the Journal of Child Neurology1.

“Understanding humor in children with autism could help us glean how they think and communicate,” says Vasudevi Reddy, professor of developmental and cultural psychology at the University of Portsmouth in the U.K. “And tuning in to what children with autism do find funny could help parents communicate and bond with their child.”

However, few studies have explored this topic in autism, and those that have, have primarily documented how children with developmental disorders laugh and smile in response to jokes or humorous situations. Some researchers hypothesize that because autism stems from impaired connections between neurons, charting how the brain processes and responds to humor may uncover some of the neurological deficits in people with autism.

In 2007, Margaret Semrud-Clikeman and colleagues launched the first brain imaging study of how children with autism process humor. In the study, the researchers are mapping which regions of the brain signal to one another as 60 children between the ages of 9 and 16 with high-functioning autism listen to jokes. The researchers plan to see how the children respond to jokes and determine how these connections change over time. They aim to complete data analysis this fall.

The researchers are also investigating whether teaching children how to identify emotions and start friendly conversations improves their understanding of humor. “We believe that we can train children with autism to develop the connections they’re missing,” says Semrud-Clikeman, professor of psychology at Michigan State University.

Barrel of laughs:

Children between 18 months and 3 years like making others laugh by teasing and clowning, or with physical humor. As their language skills develop, around age 3, they also begin to understand puns and riddles. Soon after, they learn more complex verbal humor, which requires understanding context, multiple word meanings, contradictions, metaphors and ambiguity.

Children with autism — who often lack empathy and have trouble reading social cues — develop a more limited sense of humor.

In 2002, Reddy published one of the few studies on this topic, comparing children with either autism or Down syndrome using parent reports and video recordings2. When presented with funny faces, her team found, children with either disorder laugh and smile less than typically developing children do, but children with autism laugh significantly less than do those with Down syndrome.

A 2001 functional magnetic resonance imaging study of healthy adults offers some clues about this disconnect with humor3. Researchers scanned 14 healthy participants while they listened to 60 jokes, varying from semantic ones (‘What do engineers use for birth control? Their personalities’) to puns (‘Why did the golfer wear two sets of pants? He got a hole in one’). Participants then rated how funny each joke was.

It turns out that healthy people use the brain’s left hemisphere when listening to a joke, and part of their prefrontal cortex when evaluating how funny it is. These regions, however, are impaired in autism.

“This may explain why children with autism may not laugh at certain jokes even if they understand their meaning,” says lead investigator Vinod Goel, a visiting researcher at the Max Planck Institute for Human Development in Berlin. “These children may not have the neural tools to find certain things funny.”

Other imaging studies have bolstered this idea, showing that the left hemisphere processes language and allows a person to understand the literal meaning of a joke, whereas the right hemisphere — specifically, the right frontal lobe — decodes context and subtle word plays, allowing a person to appreciate why something is funny4.

Lost in translation:

The right frontal lobe also integrates activity from the left and right hemispheres. In fact, people with damage to the right frontal lobe experience significant problems appreciating humor: they can translate a joke but cannot understand why it’s funny.

These imaging studies suggest that humor may not be processed correctly in the autistic brain. One long-standing hypothesis of autism asserts that the disorder stems from impaired long-range neural connections.

“Most of the time children with autism do not know how to use their whole brain,” notes Semrud-Clikeman. “This ability to recruit different parts of the brain quickly is important for making more complex connections and understanding more nuanced humor.”

Semrud-Clikeman says it may be possible to train children with autism to develop the long-range neural connections they may be missing.

Semrud-Clikeman cites a groundbreaking study by Sally Shaywitz, professor of pediatric neurology at Yale University School of Medicine, which showed that when children with dyslexia get intensive language training early on, they can overcome some of their learning deficits. In fact, when Shaywitz and her colleagues compared brain scans of struggling readers before and after the intensive training, they discovered that the children have improved neural connections in the brain areas associated with reading.

“My thought is, if that can happen with kids with dyslexia, there is no reason a similar intensive intervention couldn’t happen with kids with autism,” says Semrud-Clikeman.

References: